Mercurial > emacs
annotate etc/MOTIVATION @ 88054:eae3aec0f807
2008-01-29 John Wiegley <johnw@newartisans.com>
* url-auth.el (url-digest-auth): If the 'opaque' argument is not
being used, don't add it to the response text. Also, changed an
if so that the interaction between the PROMPT and OVERWRITE
arguments can no longer result in the user being queried twice for
the same login and password information.
author | John Wiegley <johnw@newartisans.com> |
---|---|
date | Tue, 29 Jan 2008 03:52:05 +0000 |
parents | 3681678d3d86 |
children |
rev | line source |
---|---|
26119 | 1 STUDIES FIND REWARD OFTEN NO MOTIVATOR |
2 | |
3 Creativity and intrinsic interest diminish if task is done for gain | |
4 | |
5 By Alfie Kohn | |
6 Special to the Boston Globe | |
7 [reprinted with permission of the author | |
8 from the Monday 19 January 1987 Boston Globe] | |
9 | |
71189
3681678d3d86
Add copyright notice and copying permissions.
Eli Zaretskii <eliz@gnu.org>
parents:
50969
diff
changeset
|
10 Verbatim copying and distribution is permitted in any medium |
3681678d3d86
Add copyright notice and copying permissions.
Eli Zaretskii <eliz@gnu.org>
parents:
50969
diff
changeset
|
11 provided this notice is preserved. |
3681678d3d86
Add copyright notice and copying permissions.
Eli Zaretskii <eliz@gnu.org>
parents:
50969
diff
changeset
|
12 |
26119 | 13 In the laboratory, rats get Rice Krispies. In the classroom the top |
14 students get A's, and in the factory or office the best workers get | |
15 raises. It's an article of faith for most of us that rewards promote | |
16 better performance. | |
17 | |
18 But a growing body of research suggests that this law is not nearly as | |
19 ironclad as was once thought. Psychologists have been finding that | |
20 rewards can lower performance levels, especially when the performance | |
21 involves creativity. | |
22 | |
23 A related series of studies shows that intrinsic interest in a task - | |
24 the sense that something is worth doing for its own sake - typically | |
25 declines when someone is rewarded for doing it. | |
26 | |
27 If a reward - money, awards, praise, or winning a contest - comes to | |
28 be seen as the reason one is engaging in an activity, that activity | |
29 will be viewed as less enjoyable in its own right. | |
30 | |
31 With the exception of some behaviorists who doubt the very existence | |
32 of intrinsic motivation, these conclusions are now widely accepted | |
33 among psychologists. Taken together, they suggest we may unwittingly | |
34 be squelching interest and discouraging innovation among workers, | |
35 students and artists. | |
36 | |
37 The recognition that rewards can have counter-productive effects is | |
38 based on a variety of studies, which have come up with such findings | |
39 as these: Young children who are rewarded for drawing are less likely | |
40 to draw on their own that are children who draw just for the fun of | |
41 it. Teenagers offered rewards for playing word games enjoy the games | |
42 less and do not do as well as those who play with no rewards. | |
43 Employees who are praised for meeting a manager's expectations suffer | |
44 a drop in motivation. | |
45 | |
46 Much of the research on creativity and motivation has been performed | |
47 by Theresa Amabile, associate professor of psychology at Brandeis | |
48 University. In a paper published early last year on her most recent | |
49 study, she reported on experiments involving elementary school and | |
50 college students. Both groups were asked to make "silly" collages. | |
51 The young children were also asked to invent stories. | |
52 | |
53 The least-creative projects, as rated by several teachers, were done | |
54 by those students who had contracted for rewards. "It may be that | |
55 commissioned work will, in general, be less creative than work that is | |
56 done out of pure interest," Amabile said. | |
57 | |
58 In 1985, Amabile asked 72 creative writers at Brandeis and at Boston | |
59 University to write poetry. Some students then were given a list of | |
60 extrinsic (external) reasons for writing, such as impressing teachers, | |
61 making money and getting into graduate school, and were asked to think | |
62 about their own writing with respect to these reasons. Others were | |
63 given a list of intrinsic reasons: the enjoyment of playing with | |
64 words, satisfaction from self-expression, and so forth. A third group | |
65 was not given any list. All were then asked to do more writing. | |
66 | |
67 The results were clear. Students given the extrinsic reasons not only | |
68 wrote less creatively than the others, as judged by 12 independent | |
69 poets, but the quality of their work dropped significantly. Rewards, | |
70 Amabile says, have this destructive effect primarily with creative | |
71 tasks, including higher-level problem-solving. "The more complex the | |
72 activity, the more it's hurt by extrinsic reward," she said. | |
73 | |
74 But other research shows that artists are by no means the only ones | |
75 affected. | |
76 | |
77 In one study, girls in the fifth and sixth grades tutored younger | |
78 children much less effectively if they were promised free movie | |
79 tickets for teaching well. The study, by James Gabarino, now | |
80 president of Chicago's Erikson Institute for Advanced Studies in Child | |
81 Development, showed that tutors working for the reward took longer to | |
82 communicate ideas, got frustrated more easily, and did a poorer job in | |
83 the end than those who were not rewarded. | |
84 | |
85 Such findings call into question the widespread belief that money is | |
86 an effective and even necessary way to motivate people. They also | |
87 challenge the behaviorist assumption that any activity is more likely | |
88 to occur if it is rewarded. Amabile says her research "definitely | |
89 refutes the notion that creativity can be operantly conditioned." | |
90 | |
91 But Kenneth McGraw, associate professor of psychology at the | |
92 University of Mississippi, cautions that this does not mean | |
93 behaviorism itself has been invalidated. "The basic principles of | |
94 reinforcement and rewards certainly work, but in a restricted context" | |
95 - restricted, that is, to tasks that are not especially interesting. | |
96 | |
97 Researchers offer several explanations for their surprising findings | |
98 about rewards and performance. | |
99 | |
100 First, rewards encourage people to focus narrowly on a task, to do it | |
101 as quickly as possible and to take few risks. "If they feel that | |
50969 | 102 'this is something I have to get through to get the prize,' they're |
26119 | 103 going to be less creative," Amabile said. |
104 | |
105 Second, people come to see themselves as being controlled by the | |
106 reward. They feel less autonomous, and this may interfere with | |
107 performance. "To the extent one's experience of being | |
108 self-determined is limited," said Richard Ryan, associate psychology | |
109 professor at the University of Rochester, "one's creativity will be | |
110 reduced as well." | |
111 | |
112 Finally, extrinsic rewards can erode intrinsic interest. People who | |
113 see themselves as working for money, approval or competitive success | |
114 find their tasks less pleasurable, and therefore do not do them as | |
115 well. | |
116 | |
117 The last explanation reflects 15 years of work by Ryan's mentor at the | |
118 University of Rochester, Edward Deci. In 1971, Deci showed that | |
119 "money may work to buy off one's intrinsic motivation for an activity" | |
120 on a long-term basis. Ten years later, Deci and his colleagues | |
121 demonstrated that trying to best others has the same effect. Students | |
122 who competed to solve a puzzle quickly were less likely than those who | |
123 were not competing to keep working at it once the experiment was over. | |
124 | |
125 Control plays role | |
126 | |
127 There is general agreement, however, that not all rewards have the | |
128 same effect. Offering a flat fee for participating in an experiment - | |
129 similar to an hourly wage in the workplace - usually does not reduce | |
130 intrinsic motivation. It is only when the rewards are based on | |
131 performing a given task or doing a good job at it - analogous to | |
132 piece-rate payment and bonuses, respectively - that the problem | |
133 develops. | |
134 | |
135 The key, then, lies in how a reward is experienced. If we come to | |
136 view ourselves as working to get something, we will no longer find | |
137 that activity worth doing in its own right. | |
138 | |
139 There is an old joke that nicely illustrates the principle. An | |
140 elderly man, harassed by the taunts of neighborhood children, finally | |
141 devises a scheme. He offered to pay each child a dollar if they would | |
142 all return Tuesday and yell their insults again. They did so eagerly | |
143 and received the money, but he told them he could only pay 25 cents on | |
144 Wednesday. When they returned, insulted him again and collected their | |
145 quarters, he informed them that Thursday's rate would be just a penny. | |
146 "Forget it," they said - and never taunted him again. | |
147 | |
148 Means to and end | |
149 | |
150 In a 1982 study, Stanford psychologist Mark L. Lepper showed that any | |
151 task, no matter how enjoyable it once seemed, would be devalued if it | |
152 were presented as a means rather than an end. He told a group of | |
153 preschoolers they could not engage in one activity they liked until | |
154 they first took part in another. Although they had enjoyed both | |
155 activities equally, the children came to dislike the task that was a | |
156 prerequisite for the other. | |
157 | |
158 It should not be surprising that when verbal feedback is experienced | |
159 as controlling, the effect on motivation can be similar to that of | |
160 payment. In a study of corporate employees, Ryan found that those who | |
161 were told, "Good, you're doing as you /should/" were "significantly | |
162 less intrinsically motivated than those who received feedback | |
163 informationally." | |
164 | |
165 There's a difference, Ryan says, between saying, "I'm giving you this | |
166 reward because I recognize the value of your work" and "You're getting | |
167 this reward because you've lived up to my standards." | |
168 | |
169 A different but related set of problems exists in the case of | |
170 creativity. Artists must make a living, of course, but Amabile | |
171 emphasizes that "the negative impact on creativity of working for | |
172 rewards can be minimized" by playing down the significance of these | |
173 rewards and trying not to use them in a controlling way. Creative | |
174 work, the research suggests, cannot be forced, but only allowed to | |
175 happen. | |
176 | |
177 /Alfie Kohn, a Cambridge, MA writer, is the author of "No Contest: The | |
178 Case Against Competition," recently published by Houghton Mifflin Co., | |
179 Boston, MA. ISBN 0-395-39387-6. / |