26119
|
1 STUDIES FIND REWARD OFTEN NO MOTIVATOR
|
|
2
|
|
3 Creativity and intrinsic interest diminish if task is done for gain
|
|
4
|
|
5 By Alfie Kohn
|
|
6 Special to the Boston Globe
|
|
7 [reprinted with permission of the author
|
|
8 from the Monday 19 January 1987 Boston Globe]
|
|
9
|
71189
|
10 Verbatim copying and distribution is permitted in any medium
|
|
11 provided this notice is preserved.
|
|
12
|
26119
|
13 In the laboratory, rats get Rice Krispies. In the classroom the top
|
|
14 students get A's, and in the factory or office the best workers get
|
|
15 raises. It's an article of faith for most of us that rewards promote
|
|
16 better performance.
|
|
17
|
|
18 But a growing body of research suggests that this law is not nearly as
|
|
19 ironclad as was once thought. Psychologists have been finding that
|
|
20 rewards can lower performance levels, especially when the performance
|
|
21 involves creativity.
|
|
22
|
|
23 A related series of studies shows that intrinsic interest in a task -
|
|
24 the sense that something is worth doing for its own sake - typically
|
|
25 declines when someone is rewarded for doing it.
|
|
26
|
|
27 If a reward - money, awards, praise, or winning a contest - comes to
|
|
28 be seen as the reason one is engaging in an activity, that activity
|
|
29 will be viewed as less enjoyable in its own right.
|
|
30
|
|
31 With the exception of some behaviorists who doubt the very existence
|
|
32 of intrinsic motivation, these conclusions are now widely accepted
|
|
33 among psychologists. Taken together, they suggest we may unwittingly
|
|
34 be squelching interest and discouraging innovation among workers,
|
|
35 students and artists.
|
|
36
|
|
37 The recognition that rewards can have counter-productive effects is
|
|
38 based on a variety of studies, which have come up with such findings
|
|
39 as these: Young children who are rewarded for drawing are less likely
|
|
40 to draw on their own that are children who draw just for the fun of
|
|
41 it. Teenagers offered rewards for playing word games enjoy the games
|
|
42 less and do not do as well as those who play with no rewards.
|
|
43 Employees who are praised for meeting a manager's expectations suffer
|
|
44 a drop in motivation.
|
|
45
|
|
46 Much of the research on creativity and motivation has been performed
|
|
47 by Theresa Amabile, associate professor of psychology at Brandeis
|
|
48 University. In a paper published early last year on her most recent
|
|
49 study, she reported on experiments involving elementary school and
|
|
50 college students. Both groups were asked to make "silly" collages.
|
|
51 The young children were also asked to invent stories.
|
|
52
|
|
53 The least-creative projects, as rated by several teachers, were done
|
|
54 by those students who had contracted for rewards. "It may be that
|
|
55 commissioned work will, in general, be less creative than work that is
|
|
56 done out of pure interest," Amabile said.
|
|
57
|
|
58 In 1985, Amabile asked 72 creative writers at Brandeis and at Boston
|
|
59 University to write poetry. Some students then were given a list of
|
|
60 extrinsic (external) reasons for writing, such as impressing teachers,
|
|
61 making money and getting into graduate school, and were asked to think
|
|
62 about their own writing with respect to these reasons. Others were
|
|
63 given a list of intrinsic reasons: the enjoyment of playing with
|
|
64 words, satisfaction from self-expression, and so forth. A third group
|
|
65 was not given any list. All were then asked to do more writing.
|
|
66
|
|
67 The results were clear. Students given the extrinsic reasons not only
|
|
68 wrote less creatively than the others, as judged by 12 independent
|
|
69 poets, but the quality of their work dropped significantly. Rewards,
|
|
70 Amabile says, have this destructive effect primarily with creative
|
|
71 tasks, including higher-level problem-solving. "The more complex the
|
|
72 activity, the more it's hurt by extrinsic reward," she said.
|
|
73
|
|
74 But other research shows that artists are by no means the only ones
|
|
75 affected.
|
|
76
|
|
77 In one study, girls in the fifth and sixth grades tutored younger
|
|
78 children much less effectively if they were promised free movie
|
|
79 tickets for teaching well. The study, by James Gabarino, now
|
|
80 president of Chicago's Erikson Institute for Advanced Studies in Child
|
|
81 Development, showed that tutors working for the reward took longer to
|
|
82 communicate ideas, got frustrated more easily, and did a poorer job in
|
|
83 the end than those who were not rewarded.
|
|
84
|
|
85 Such findings call into question the widespread belief that money is
|
|
86 an effective and even necessary way to motivate people. They also
|
|
87 challenge the behaviorist assumption that any activity is more likely
|
|
88 to occur if it is rewarded. Amabile says her research "definitely
|
|
89 refutes the notion that creativity can be operantly conditioned."
|
|
90
|
|
91 But Kenneth McGraw, associate professor of psychology at the
|
|
92 University of Mississippi, cautions that this does not mean
|
|
93 behaviorism itself has been invalidated. "The basic principles of
|
|
94 reinforcement and rewards certainly work, but in a restricted context"
|
|
95 - restricted, that is, to tasks that are not especially interesting.
|
|
96
|
|
97 Researchers offer several explanations for their surprising findings
|
|
98 about rewards and performance.
|
|
99
|
|
100 First, rewards encourage people to focus narrowly on a task, to do it
|
|
101 as quickly as possible and to take few risks. "If they feel that
|
50969
|
102 'this is something I have to get through to get the prize,' they're
|
26119
|
103 going to be less creative," Amabile said.
|
|
104
|
|
105 Second, people come to see themselves as being controlled by the
|
|
106 reward. They feel less autonomous, and this may interfere with
|
|
107 performance. "To the extent one's experience of being
|
|
108 self-determined is limited," said Richard Ryan, associate psychology
|
|
109 professor at the University of Rochester, "one's creativity will be
|
|
110 reduced as well."
|
|
111
|
|
112 Finally, extrinsic rewards can erode intrinsic interest. People who
|
|
113 see themselves as working for money, approval or competitive success
|
|
114 find their tasks less pleasurable, and therefore do not do them as
|
|
115 well.
|
|
116
|
|
117 The last explanation reflects 15 years of work by Ryan's mentor at the
|
|
118 University of Rochester, Edward Deci. In 1971, Deci showed that
|
|
119 "money may work to buy off one's intrinsic motivation for an activity"
|
|
120 on a long-term basis. Ten years later, Deci and his colleagues
|
|
121 demonstrated that trying to best others has the same effect. Students
|
|
122 who competed to solve a puzzle quickly were less likely than those who
|
|
123 were not competing to keep working at it once the experiment was over.
|
|
124
|
|
125 Control plays role
|
|
126
|
|
127 There is general agreement, however, that not all rewards have the
|
|
128 same effect. Offering a flat fee for participating in an experiment -
|
|
129 similar to an hourly wage in the workplace - usually does not reduce
|
|
130 intrinsic motivation. It is only when the rewards are based on
|
|
131 performing a given task or doing a good job at it - analogous to
|
|
132 piece-rate payment and bonuses, respectively - that the problem
|
|
133 develops.
|
|
134
|
|
135 The key, then, lies in how a reward is experienced. If we come to
|
|
136 view ourselves as working to get something, we will no longer find
|
|
137 that activity worth doing in its own right.
|
|
138
|
|
139 There is an old joke that nicely illustrates the principle. An
|
|
140 elderly man, harassed by the taunts of neighborhood children, finally
|
|
141 devises a scheme. He offered to pay each child a dollar if they would
|
|
142 all return Tuesday and yell their insults again. They did so eagerly
|
|
143 and received the money, but he told them he could only pay 25 cents on
|
|
144 Wednesday. When they returned, insulted him again and collected their
|
|
145 quarters, he informed them that Thursday's rate would be just a penny.
|
|
146 "Forget it," they said - and never taunted him again.
|
|
147
|
|
148 Means to and end
|
|
149
|
|
150 In a 1982 study, Stanford psychologist Mark L. Lepper showed that any
|
|
151 task, no matter how enjoyable it once seemed, would be devalued if it
|
|
152 were presented as a means rather than an end. He told a group of
|
|
153 preschoolers they could not engage in one activity they liked until
|
|
154 they first took part in another. Although they had enjoyed both
|
|
155 activities equally, the children came to dislike the task that was a
|
|
156 prerequisite for the other.
|
|
157
|
|
158 It should not be surprising that when verbal feedback is experienced
|
|
159 as controlling, the effect on motivation can be similar to that of
|
|
160 payment. In a study of corporate employees, Ryan found that those who
|
|
161 were told, "Good, you're doing as you /should/" were "significantly
|
|
162 less intrinsically motivated than those who received feedback
|
|
163 informationally."
|
|
164
|
|
165 There's a difference, Ryan says, between saying, "I'm giving you this
|
|
166 reward because I recognize the value of your work" and "You're getting
|
|
167 this reward because you've lived up to my standards."
|
|
168
|
|
169 A different but related set of problems exists in the case of
|
|
170 creativity. Artists must make a living, of course, but Amabile
|
|
171 emphasizes that "the negative impact on creativity of working for
|
|
172 rewards can be minimized" by playing down the significance of these
|
|
173 rewards and trying not to use them in a controlling way. Creative
|
|
174 work, the research suggests, cannot be forced, but only allowed to
|
|
175 happen.
|
|
176
|
|
177 /Alfie Kohn, a Cambridge, MA writer, is the author of "No Contest: The
|
|
178 Case Against Competition," recently published by Houghton Mifflin Co.,
|
|
179 Boston, MA. ISBN 0-395-39387-6. /
|